The Joumal of Neuroscience,January 1,1997,17(1):353-362 Human Brain Language Areas Identified by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Jeffrey R.Binder,12 Julie A.Frost,Thomas A.Hammeke,Robert W.Cox,3 Stephen M.Rao,2 and Thomas Prieto Departments of Neurology and 2Cellular Biology and Anatomy,and Biophysics Research Institute,Medical College of Wisconsin,Milwaukee,Wisconsin 53226 areas i the i I lobes I onsistent with classical r antic functi y to in the ory,mo and general executive functic ns The I nguage middle temporal,inferior temporal,fusiform,and angular gyri ed with a contro areas in a tas handed subiects.These maps were averaged in standard ste otaxic space to produ av or for ongoing cassCalmodesf1angageorganiaionAasedcnesion Language-related functions were among the first to be ascribed a Functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI)is one such on moni 100 mode of organization.based on data and rema s in co mmon use(Wemicke.17:Lichth Mayeux and Kandel, 1985 Binder et DeYoe et:Sereno et a1995) sive"area for planning and executing nctions o ha eech and writing m of sed an ts,nam d atter Broca (B the reliability of respones in these proc cedures remain unc accept this Sha witz et al. 1995) ed ot univ tic about aurall to clicit receptive anguage pro ciative of new data on languag with the goal as mar candidat (Petersen et al.,1988:Demonet et al..1992;Bottini et al.,1994) control for activation of early auditory ed Aur.1 1996:revised Oct.7.1996: ed0t.11,1996. tion and ar gical D natio J.Hyde.A.Je nd l a strong correlation en this s de otid amobarbital procedure(Binder et al. 1995.199 6b).Thes d in maging ion p
Human Brain Language Areas Identified by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Jeffrey R. Binder,1,2 Julie A. Frost,1 Thomas A. Hammeke,1 Robert W. Cox,3 Stephen M. Rao,1,2 and Thomas Prieto1 Departments of 1Neurology and 2Cellular Biology and Anatomy, and 3Biophysics Research Institute, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53226 Functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) was used to identify candidate language processing areas in the intact human brain. Language was defined broadly to include both phonological and lexical–semantic functions and to exclude sensory, motor, and general executive functions. The language activation task required phonetic and semantic analysis of aurally presented words and was compared with a control task involving perceptual analysis of nonlinguistic sounds. Functional maps of the entire brain were obtained from 30 righthanded subjects. These maps were averaged in standard stereotaxic space to produce a robust “average activation map” that proved reliable in a split-half analysis. As predicted from classical models of language organization based on lesion data, cortical activation associated with language processing was strongly lateralized to the left cerebral hemisphere and involved a network of regions in the frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes. Less consistent with classical models were (1) the existence of left hemisphere temporoparietal language areas outside the traditional “Wernicke area,” namely, in the middle temporal, inferior temporal, fusiform, and angular gyri; (2) extensive left prefrontal language areas outside the classical “Broca area”; and (3) clear participation of these left frontal areas in a task emphasizing “receptive” language functions. Although partly in conflict with the classical model of language localization, these findings are generally compatible with reported lesion data and provide additional support for ongoing efforts to refine and extend the classical model. Key words: language; functional magnetic resonance imaging; brain mapping; semantic; phonological; auditory cortex Language-related functions were among the first to be ascribed a specific location in the human brain (Broca, 1861) and have been the subject of intense research for well over a century. A “classical model” of language organization, based on data from aphasic patients with brain lesions, was popularized during the late 19th century and remains in common use (Wernicke, 1874; Lichtheim, 1885; Geschwind, 1971; Benson, 1985; Mayeux and Kandel, 1985). In its most general form, this model proposes a frontal, “expressive” area for planning and executing speech and writing movements, named after Broca (Broca, 1861), and a posterior, “receptive” area for analysis and identification of linguistic sensory stimuli, named after Wernicke (Wernicke, 1874). Although many researchers would accept this basic scheme, a more detailed account of language organization has not yet gained widespread approval. There is not universal agreement, for example, on such basic issues as which cortical areas make up the receptive language system (Bogen and Bogen, 1976) or on the specific linguistic role of Broca’s area (Marie, 1906; Mohr, 1976). Noninvasive functional imaging methods are a potential source of new data on language organization in the intact human brain (Petersen et al., 1988; De´monet et al., 1992; Bottini et al., 1994). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) is one such method, which is based on monitoring regional changes in blood oxygenation resulting from neural activity (Ogawa et al., 1990, 1992). Although certain technical issues remain to be resolved, the capabilities of FMRI for localizing primary sensory and motor areas are now well established (Kim et al., 1993; Rao et al., 1993; Binder et al., 1994b; DeYoe et al., 1994; Sereno et al., 1995). Preliminary studies of higher cognitive functions also have been reported, but the validity of the activation procedures used and the reliability of responses in these procedures remain unclear (Hinke et al., 1993; McCarthy et al., 1993; Cohen et al., 1994; Rueckert et al., 1994; Binder et al., 1995; Demb et al., 1995; Shaywitz et al., 1995). We used FMRI to investigate the cortical regions involved in language processing in normal, right-handed subjects. The linguistic task, which required meaning-based decisions about aurally presented words, was designed to elicit receptive language processing at both phonetic (speech perceptual) and semantic (associative) levels, with the goal of identifying as many candidate “receptive language” areas as possible. A baseline task, which required pitch-based decisions about tone sequences, was used to control for activation of early auditory processors, nonspecific executive functions mediating attention and arousal, and motor response systems. Preliminary studies using this language task demonstrated left hemisphere lateralization of blood oxygenation responses and a strong correlation between this lateralization measure and language lateralization as determined by the intracarotid amobarbital procedure (Binder et al., 1995, 1996b). These preliminary studies investigated individual subject activation patterns only and did not include imaging of medial brain regions. A Received Aug. 1, 1996; revised Oct. 7, 1996; accepted Oct. 11, 1996. This work was supported by a grant from the McDonnell-Pew Program in Cognitive Neuroscience, National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Stroke Grant RO1 NS33576, and National Institute of Mental Health Grant PO1 MH51358. We thank J. Hyde, A. Jesmanowicz, W. O’Reilly, and L. Estkowski for discussion and technical assistance. Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. J. R. Binder, Department of Neurology, Medical College of Wisconsin, 9200 West Wisconsin Avenue, Milwaukee, WI 53226. Copyright q 1996 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/96/170353-10$05.00/0 The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1, 1997, 17(1):353–362
354 J.Neurosci.January 1.197,17()56 not the fo of this s(C g pattern and checked the in with the classical model ofn ms:and (2)acc Damasio et al.1996)but a (Damas ach tone in ce and 100 et Hart and Gordon,19 Hillis and Caramazza,1991 1996),suggesting a 750Hz MATERIALS AND METHODS had thumb to a hand-held igh mm o at ere obta eea k the spa .This MR) Each imas of the comp Sound al four ed pl: The mackgr r i SPI 1100d "drif or the r ed and of rtainin eCW-AFNI S (SPGR)of Tal
more recent report presented averaged group activation patterns but only for the lateral third of the left hemisphere (Binder et al., 1996a). In the present study, we imaged the entire brain in 30 subjects, merged the spatially normalized data to obtain an average activation pattern, and checked the reliability of this pattern by comparing two matched subgroups from the original sample. As in previous functional imaging studies, the findings are partly in conflict with the classical model of language localization (Petersen et al., 1988; De´monet et al., 1992; Bottini et al., 1994; Damasio et al., 1996) but are generally compatible with a large body of recent lesion data (Damasio, 1981; Kertesz et al., 1982; Freedman et al., 1984; Warrington and Shallice, 1984; Alexander et al., 1989; Hart and Gordon, 1990; Hillis and Caramazza, 1991; Rapcsak and Rubens, 1994; Damasio et al., 1996), suggesting a need to refine and extend the classical model. MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects. Subjects were 30 healthy adults (15 women, 15 men), ranging in age from 18 to 29 years, with no history of neurological, psychiatric, or auditory symptoms. Edinburgh Handedness Inventory laterality quotients (Oldfield, 1971) ranged from 58 to 100, indicating strong right-hand preferences for all subjects. Subjects were recruited on a voluntary basis, gave written informed consent, and were paid a small hourly stipend. All studies received prior approval by the Medical College of Wisconsin Human Research Review Committee. Apparatus and scanning procedures. Scanning was conducted at 1.5 Tesla on a General Electric (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI) Signa scanner using a three-axis local gradient coil with an insertable transmit– receive radiofrequency coil optimized for whole-brain echo-planar imaging (EPI). Functional imaging used a gradient-echo EPI sequence with the following parameters: 40 msec echo time, 4 sec repetition time, 24 cm field of view, 64 3 64 pixel matrix, and 3.75 3 3.75 3 7.0 mm voxel dimensions. Seventeen to 19 contiguous sagittal slice locations were imaged, encompassing the entire brain. One-hundred sequential images were collected at each of the slice locations. High-resolution, T1- weighted anatomical reference images were obtained as a set of 124 contiguous sagittal slices using a three-dimensional spoiled-gradient-echo sequence (SPGR, GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI). Subjects were scanned with eyes closed and room lights dimmed. Padding was placed behind the neck and around the head as needed to relax the cervical spine and to pack the space between the head and inner surface of the coil. This padding minimizes the range of motion that can occur and provides tactile feedback for subjects who are attempting to remain motionless. Each 100-image EPI series began with four baseline images (16 sec) to allow magnetic resonance (MR) signal to reach equilibrium, followed by 96 images during which two comparison conditions were alternated every 24 sec. Each image series thus consisted of eight task alternation cycles (12 images/cycle). Stimuli and activation tasks. Stimuli were 16-bit digitally synthesized tones and sampled male speech sounds presented binaurally at precise intervals using a computer playback system. Sounds were amplified near the scanner using a magnetically shielded transducer system and were delivered to the subject via air conduction through 180 cm paired plastic tubes. The tubes were threaded through tightly occlusive ear inserts that attenuated background scanner noise to ;75 dB sound pressure level (SPL). Background scanner noise was constant throughout all rest and task conditions. Intensity of the experimental stimuli averaged 100 dB SPL and remained constant across all subjects and all stimuli. Subjects reported that the experimental stimuli were subjectively louder than the background scanner noise. Comparison conditions included an unspecified “rest” state and two explicit behavioral tasks. For the rest condition, subjects were instructed to remain relaxed and motionless but were given no other instructions or stimuli. The behavioral tasks were designed to direct subjects’ attention either to physical characteristics of nonlinguistic stimuli (“tone decision”) or to semantically related information pertaining to linguistic stimuli (“semantic decision”). The rationale for these tasks was given previously (Binder et al., 1995) and will be briefly summarized. First, it is hypothesized that a variety of general-purpose, nonlinguistic functional systems are activated during most language behaviors. These could include early sensory processors, motor systems, short-term memory systems, and attention-arousal networks. Although vital to language behavior, these systems are not the focus of interest of this study insofar as they are also activated during nonlinguistic behaviors. Second, it is known that a considerable amount of “automatic” processing of linguistic stimuli takes place at phonological and semantic levels regardless of the behavioral situation in which stimulus presentation occurs (Carr et al., 1982; Marcel, 1983; Van Orden, 1987; Price et al., 1996). The nonlinguistic comparison task used in this study (tone decision) was therefore designed to (1) control for activation of sensory, motor, and general-purpose executive systems; and (2) accomplish this with minimal “automatic” activation of language systems. Stimuli in the tone decision task were 500 and 750 Hz pure tones, with a duration of 150 msec and 5 msec onset and offset envelopes. These were presented as sequences of three to seven tones, with 250 msec of silence separating each tone in a sequence and 1000 msec separating each sequence. Sequences contained an average of five tones, resulting in an average total stimulus duration of 750 msec. Subjects were required to respond by button press for any sequence containing two 750 Hz tones. Stimuli in the semantic decision task were spoken English nouns designating animals (e.g., “turtle”). These words had an average usage frequency of 9.3 per million (SD 26.2, range 0 –203) (Francis and Kucera, 1982) and contained an average of 5.2 phonemes (SD 2.0, range 2–10). Each animal word was used only once during the entire imaging session. Stimuli were edited to an average duration of 750 msec and were followed by a 2250 msec interstimulus interval. Subjects were required to respond by button press for animals they considered to be both “native to the United States” and “used by humans.” The tasks were matched on stimulus intensity, average stimulus duration per trial (750 msec), average trial duration (3 sec), and frequency of positive targets (1 target/8 sec). Responses consisted of a thumb press to a hand-held button device placed in the subject’s left hand. Button responses produced a visual signal in the control room that was used to log performance accuracy. Two functional image series were acquired. In the tone decision–rest comparison, subjects performed the tone decision task eight times, with eight intervening epochs of rest. In the semantic decision–tone decision comparison, subjects performed the semantic decision task eight times with eight intervening periods of tone decision. Instructions and brief practice trials on each task were given before scanning. Data analysis. To compensate for artifactual signal intensity changes caused by head movement, a modified version of the automated image registration program developed by Woods et al. (Woods et al., 1992) was used to register the EPI images within each time series. This program uses an iterative procedure to minimize the variance in voxel intensity ratios of two images. In the present study, images 5–100 in each time series were registered with image 4, and only these images were used in additional analyses. Each registered 96-image series was then viewed as a cine loop to detect residual visible head motion. Images showing such motion made up ,2% of the total acquired data and were not included in subsequent analyses. No 96-image series contained more than four motion-contaminated images. For each functional time series, t tests were conducted at each voxel to measure changes in signal intensity between comparison conditions. For the purposes of this analysis, one of the comparison conditions was designated the activation, or “probe,” condition and the other the “control” condition. First, the final four images within each of the eight probe epochs were averaged to produce an image of average signal intensity values during the last 12 sec of each probe epoch. This procedure ensured that the measured values would reflect steady-state activation levels after completion of the hemodynamic response (Binder et al., 1995). Next, the final four images obtained during control epochs before and after each probe epoch were averaged, and a probe-control difference image was created for each activation cycle by subtracting the average control image from the corresponding average probe image (Binder et al., 1994a). This use of a “local baseline” lessens the effects of signal instability caused by scanner “drift” and low-frequency spontaneous oscillations. Finally, these mean probe-control difference values were compared, on a voxel-by-voxel basis, against a hypothetical mean of zero using pooled-variance Student’s t tests. This procedure generates statistical parametric maps (SPMs) of t deviates reflecting differences between probe and control states at each voxel location for each subject. For the sake of simplicity, such differences are referred to below as “activation.” Individual anatomical (SPGR) scans and SPMs were then transformed into the standard stereotaxic space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988), using the MCW-AFNI software package (Cox, 1996). This procedure involves marking fiducial points on the high-resolution anatomical scans at the anterior commissure, posterior commissure, midsagittal plane, and 354 J. Neurosci., January 1, 1997, 17(1):353–362 Binder et al. • FMRI of Language Areas
Binder时al.·FMRI of Language Area .Neurosci,.January1.1997.17353-36235 nsula and did not spread to adiacent insular cortex In both (P ver Anat of the p r)cortex on eithe side of FNI softw h 196)ane rcularis,por tivate in the nting the motor a (SMA)b e of this us laterally with the act ally se frontal lobe reas wa hat more extensive in the righ ntral s to mp the left hand nidbrain ng each s will be the subject of a sub The posterio was activated bilat RESULTS the tone task was ated with bilateral activa Task performance STO reand tored te nnd All subjects I u.The rights is an average score of 983 correct (range,89 -100%) wiy cach sphere only right-handed controls on th Language areas itha probability ded t 时是 were found almost Tone-task activation ted cortex on both sides of the STS and most of the MTG i rus (G)and fusiform an were act ated bilaterally.This activation extended throughou task than to the semantic task(ie showed relative sign
brain edges. To compensate for normal variation in anatomy across subjects (Toga et al., 1993), the unthresholded, stereotaxically resampled three-dimensional SPMs were smoothed slightly with a Gaussian filter of root-mean-square radius 4 mm. These data sets were then merged across subjects by averaging the t statistics in each voxel. The procedure of averaging statistics was chosen to guard against nonequal MR signal variances among subjects. This heteroscedasticity could arise from many causes; differing degrees of subject motion, differing amounts of cardiacinduced tissue pulsatility, and variability in the scanner between sessions are three likely sources. The averaged t statistics were then thresholded to identify voxels in which the mean change in MR signal between comparison conditions was unlikely to be zero. The average of a set of t deviates is not a tabulated distribution. Therefore, the Cornish–Fisher expansion of the inverse distribution of a sum of random deviates was used to select a threshold for rejection of the null hypothesis (Fisher and Cornish, 1960). Only average t scores $0.875 were considered significant ( p , 0.0001). Individual three-dimensional SPGR data from the 30 subjects were also merged to produce an “average brain” for anatomical reference. Anatomical labels for activated areas were derived by interactive threedimensional inspection of stereotaxically registered functional and anatomical data using MCW-AFNI software (Cox, 1996) and by reference to the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (1988). Activated regions were typically large expanses of cortex following gyral and sulcal topography rather than small foci and were thus more completely described by reference to anatomical structures than in terms of point coordinates. Activation areas were given anatomical labels only when the borders of the area followed borders of a gyral or sulcal structure in the Talairach and Tournoux atlas and the label was supported by three-dimensional inspection of the averaged anatomical data. A split-half procedure was used to estimate how well the semantic decision–tone decision activation pattern would generalize to other subject samples, analogous to the use of this procedure in test theory for estimating item homogeneity (Crocker and Algina, 1986). Subjects were divided into two independent samples of 15 subjects each, matched on gender, age, and handedness. Individual subject SPMs from the semantic decision–tone decision comparison were averaged within each subgroup. These average SPMs were then correlated on a voxel-by-voxel basis after masking voxels in nonbrain regions of the image, yielding a reliability coefficient for the two halves of the total sample. A reliability coefficient for the activation pattern obtained from the entire sample of 30 subjects was then estimated using the Spearman–Brown prophecy formula, rXX 5 2rAB/(1 1 rAB), where rXX is the estimated reliability coefficient for the entire sample, and rAB is the split-half correlation (Spearman, 1910). Finally, a visual comparison of the location of activation peaks in each subgroup was made after thresholding each subgroup SPM at an average t score of 1.30 ( p , 0.0001). RESULTS Task performance All subjects learned the tasks easily and tolerated the scanning procedure well. Performance on the tone decision task was uniformly good, with subjects attaining an average score of 98.3% correct (range, 89 –100%). Responses by each subject on the semantic decision task were compared with those given by a group of 50 normal right-handed controls on the same stimulus set. Items responded to with a probability .0.75 by controls were categorized as targets, and items responded to with a probability ,0.25 by controls were categorized as foils. Subjects’ percent correct scores in discriminating targets from foils averaged 92.6% (range, 73–100%). Tone-task activation Compared with the resting state, performance of the tone task produced blood oxygenation changes in multiple areas of both cerebral hemispheres, cerebellum bilaterally, bilateral deep nuclei, and brainstem (Fig. 1). The superior temporal gyri (STG) were activated bilaterally. This activation extended throughout much of the STG, including Heschl’s (transverse temporal) gyrus, the superior temporal plane posterior and anterior to Heschl’s gyrus, and much of the lateral surface of the STG excluding cortex near the anterior aspect of the superior temporal sulcus (STS). Activation in the STG did not spread ventrally beyond the STS in the left hemisphere but did so in the right hemisphere, involving the posterior half of the middle temporal gyrus (MTG). Activation of the superior temporal plane anterior to Heschl’s gyrus (planum polare) did not include temporal cortex adjacent to the insula and did not spread to adjacent insular cortex. In both hemispheres, activation spread posteriorly along the planum temporale and the lower bank of the posterior ascending ramus of the sylvian fissure (planum parietale) and into the surrounding supramarginal gyrus. This activation of the supramarginal gyrus was much more extensive in the right hemisphere. Activation also involved the inferior, middle, and superior frontal gyri bilaterally. In the left hemisphere, this followed the length of the precentral sulcus, involving (premotor) cortex on either side of this sulcus. Right hemisphere frontal activation was also observed along the length of the precentral sulcus but also extended anteriorly to involve the pars opercularis, portions of the pars triangularis, and prefrontal cortex along the inferior frontal sulcus and in the middle frontal gyrus. In both hemispheres, there was activation of anterior insular cortex underneath the activated region of frontal operculum. Activation of the superior frontal gyrus involved a region just anterior to the paracentral lobule, probably representing the supplementary motor area (SMA) bilaterally (x 5 66, y 5 25, z 5 61). In both hemispheres, this SMA focus was contiguous laterally with the activation centered on the precentral sulcus and was contiguous ventrally with an area of activation in the midanterior cingulate gyrus. Activation in all these frontal lobe areas was somewhat more extensive in the right hemisphere. A small activation focus was present in the region of the right central sulcus, approximately at the site of motor representation for the left hand. Subcortical activation was observed bilaterally in the anterior putamen, genu of the internal capsule, thalamus, and dorsal midbrain. Activation in these structures was also somewhat more prominent on the right. Details of these subcortical activations will be the subject of a subsequent report. The posterior and lateral cerebellum was activated bilaterally, more extensively on the left. In summary, the tone task was associated with bilateral activation of multiple cortical areas including primary and association auditory cortex of the STG, supramarginal gyrus, premotor cortex and SMA, anterior cingulate, and anterior insula. The right supramarginal gyrus was clearly more active than the left. The MTG and several prefrontal areas were activated in the right hemisphere only. Language areas Areas that were activated more strongly by the semantic decision task than by the tone task were defined as language areas and were found almost exclusively in the left hemisphere or in the right cerebellum (Fig. 2). In contrast to the tone task, which activated the left STG but not MTG, the semantic decision task activated cortex on both sides of the STS and most of the MTG in the left hemisphere. This activation also spread ventrally across portions of the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) and fusiform and parahippocampal gyri in the ventral temporal lobe. In contrast, several other temporal lobe areas responded more strongly to the tone task than to the semantic task (i.e., showed relative signal “decreases” during the semantic task). These included the planum temporale bilaterally and the posterior MTG in the right hemiBinder et al. • FMRI of Language Areas J. Neurosci., January 1, 1997, 17(1):353–362 355
350J.Nerosci..January1.1997.170)353-36 Figure 1 L60 L54 L48 L4 iaure 2 104 00 0 10- 10-6 10-8
356 J. Neurosci., January 1, 1997, 17(1):353–362 Binder et al. • FMRI of Language Areas
Binder eal.·MRl of Language Area Neurosci.,January 1.1997.17(1k353-362 35 level of activation by the tone and semantic tasks. lariiparsotbitalsandcortexaloathcntcriorfontalslcu5 Reliability of language activation pattems and media tiond and strick. and the level of correlation that would be expected between the activatior much angular gyrus activatior handedness,and gender hat had Like the pla num tem DISCUSSION activated by the anti cortex,and isph tive to the tone task incl a portio the cau inguistic stimuli(single words),there may also have been auto of the stimuli.such as those pertaining to orthog aphie and erior right cerebe mu sma ler activation was ob- .By comparing this task with a nonlar ve-rela ted areas we ing,maint nd ubtra form and par anguage proces inte atio n tha astudy owing very cl dence his FMri la dat angul y and (4) d from ntrac region ng po Talairach .1988 ng the m.On clate the nd Tou a The bl ask is de cd in the tkativ to the semantis d por and MTG p)The let right (1 driehtMTGWeeactivatedmoretongtheonede
sphere. The other bilateral STG (auditory) areas that had been activated by the tone task relative to rest showed no difference in level of activation by the tone and semantic tasks. Virtually the entire inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) was activated by the semantic decision task, including pars opercularis, pars triangularis, pars orbitalis, and cortex along the inferior frontal sulcus. Rostral and caudal areas of the middle frontal gyrus were active, whereas the midportion of this gyrus (approximately Brodmann area 9) was not. Much of the superior frontal gyrus anterior to the vertical AC line demonstrated activation, including the pre-SMA and medial aspect of Brodmann areas 8 –10 (Picard and Strick, 1996). Left medial frontal activation also spread ventrally to involve part of the anterior cingulate gyrus. This overlapped but was mostly rostral to the cingulate region activated by the tone task. Much smaller anterior cingulate and superior frontal gyrus activations were observed in the right hemisphere. The SMA and premotor cortex along the precentral sulcus were not activated by the semantic task more than the tone task; premotor cortex of the right hemisphere and a small area of dorsolateral premotor cortex in the left hemisphere responded more strongly to the tone task. A third major focus of activation by the semantic task was in the left angular gyrus. A much smaller angular gyrus activation was observed in the right hemisphere. These areas were immediately posterior to the planum temporale and supramarginal gyrus foci that had been activated by the tone task. Like the planum temporale, the supramarginal gyrus in both hemispheres was more strongly activated by the tone task than the semantic task. A fourth large cortical region activated by the semantic task involved the posterior cingulate gyrus, a portion of the precuneus, retrosplenial cortex, and cingulate isthmus in the left hemisphere. A much smaller retrosplenial activation was present in the right hemisphere. Deep structures activated by the semantic task relative to the tone task included a portion of the caudate nucleus, anterior internal capsule, and anterior thalamus in the left hemisphere only. Semantic task activation involved a large part of the posterior right cerebellum. A much smaller activation was observed near the posterior midline of the left cerebellum. In summary, four distinct cortical language-related areas were observed in the left hemisphere. These were: (1) a lateral and ventral temporal lobe region that included STS, MTG, and parts of the ITG and fusiform and parahippocampal gyri; (2) a prefrontal region that included much of the inferior and superior frontal gyri, rostral and caudal aspects of the middle frontal gyrus, and a portion of the anterior cingulate; (3) angular gyrus; and (4) a perisplenial region including posterior cingulate, ventromedial precuneus, and cingulate isthmus. These regions were clearly distinct from auditory, premotor, SMA, and supramarginal gyrus areas that had been bilaterally activated by the tone task. The other large region activated by the semantic task was the right posterior cerebellum. Reliability of language activation patterns The pattern of language activation observed in the group average was also visible in the individual subjects’ data, as exemplified by the subject shown in Figure 3. Language activation patterns were very similar in the two matched samples of 15 subjects (Fig. 4A,B). Although the overall level of activation was somewhat greater in subgroup 2 (Fig. 4B), the four left hemisphere language regions in the temporal lobe, frontal lobe, angular gyrus, and perisplenial cortex were clearly evident in both subgroups, as was the focus in right posterior cerebellum. Voxel-by-voxel correlation between the activation maps from the two subgroups was 0.86, and the Spearman–Brown estimated reliability coefficient for the entire sample of 30 subjects was 0.92. This result indicates the level of correlation that would be expected between the activation pattern from this sample and activation patterns from other random samples of 30 subjects matched to this sample on age, handedness, and gender. DISCUSSION This FMRI study sought to identify candidate language processing areas in the intact human brain and to distinguish these from nonlanguage areas. The language activation task emphasized perceptual analysis of speech sounds (“phonetic processing”) and retrieval of previously learned verbal information associated with the speech sounds (“semantic processing”). Because this task used linguistic stimuli (single words), there may also have been automatic activation of other neural codes related to linguistic aspects of the stimuli, such as those pertaining to orthographic and syntactic representations. By comparing this task with a nonlanguage control task, areas activated equally by both tasks, such as those involved in low-level auditory processing, maintenance of attention, and response production, were “subtracted” from the resulting activation map, revealing areas likely to be involved in language processing. Empirical support for this interpretation comes from a study showing very close correspondence between this FMRI language measure and language lateralization data obtained from intracarotid amobarbital injection (Binder et al., 1996b). The observed language activation pattern appears to be 4 Figure 1. Top. FMRI activation map for the tone decision–rest comparison. The data are presented as sequential sagittal sections from left to right, with the stereotaxic coordinate x-axis, L–R given for each section (see Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). The anteroposterior commissural line ( y-axis) and vertical AC line (z-axis) are shown in green. Activated voxels are shown superimposed on stereotaxically averaged anatomical brain images. Probability values for these voxels are coded using the color scale at bottom. One centimeter tick marks are provided to help relate the large activation regions to Talairach and Tournoux (1988). Structures showing bilateral activation include the STG and planum temporale, the supramarginal gyrus, premotor cortex and SMA, midanterior cingulate gyrus, anterior insula, anterior putamen, thalamus, midbrain, and posterior cerebellum. Modest right hemisphere lateralization is evidenced by right unilateral activation in the IFG and MTG and by relative rightward asymmetry in most of the other activated regions. Figure 2. Bottom. FMRI activation map for the semantic decision–tone decision comparison. The data are formatted as in Figure 1. The blue-cyan color scale codes probability values for voxels activated by the tone decision task relative to the semantic decision task. Left temporal lobe activation by the language task is demonstrated in the STS and MTG (L60-48), ITG (L54-48), fusiform gyrus (L42-30), and parahippocampus (L30-18). The left angular gyrus is activated over a large region (L54-30). Left frontal activation involves the entire IFG (L54-36), rostral and caudal (but not central) middle frontal gyrus (L42-24), and superior frontal gyrus rostral to the SMA (L24-6). On the medial wall, there is activation spreading into anterior cingulate (L6) and a focus involving retrosplenial cortex and neighboring precuneus (L12-6). The posterior right cerebellum is activated by the semantic decision task (R6-42). Small language activation foci are noted in the left anterior thalamus and caudate, left medial cerebellum, right retrosplenial region, and right angular gyrus. Portions of the premotor cortex, planum temporale, supramarginal gyrus, and right MTG were activated more strongly by the tone decision (control) task (i.e., showed relative signal “decreases” during the semantic decision task). Binder et al. • FMRI of Language Areas J. Neurosci., January 1, 1997, 17(1):353–362 357